INFORMACIÓN

La revista Psicothema fue fundada en Asturias en 1989 y está editada conjuntamente por la Facultad y el Departamento de Psicología de la Universidad de Oviedo y el Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos del Principado de Asturias. Publica cuatro números al año.
Se admiten trabajos tanto de investigación básica como aplicada, pertenecientes a cualquier ámbito de la Psicología, que previamente a su publicación son evaluados anónimamente por revisores externos.

PSICOTHEMA
  • Director: Laura E. Gómez Sánchez
  • Periodicidad:
         Febrero | Mayo | Agosto | Noviembre
  • ISSN: 0214-9915
  • ISSN Electrónico: 1886-144X
CONTACTO
  • Dirección: Ildelfonso Sánchez del Río, 4, 1º B
    33001 Oviedo (España)
  • Teléfono: 985 285 778
  • Fax:985 281 374
  • Email: psicothema@cop.es

Psicothema, 2001. Vol. Vol. 13 (nº 1). 57-62




SUBJECTIVE AND AUTONOMIC STRESS RESPONSES IN ALEXITHYMIA

Francisco Martínez-Sánchez, Beatriz Ortiz-Soria and Manuel Ato-García

University of Murcia

Alexithymia refers to a specific disturbance in emotional processing that is manifested through difficulties in identifying and verbalizing feelings. The main objective of this investigation has been the study about the relationship between subjective and autonomic physiological reactivity pattern to stressful laboratory situation, related with alexithymia level, assessed by the Toronto Alexithymia Scale, TAS-20. The experiment involved six phases: I-adaptation, II-relaxation, III-stress (mental arithmetic), IV-relaxation, V-stress (watching a distressing film), and VI-relaxation. During all periods, the subjective self-perception of physiological activation and autonomic reactivity (using the Palmar Sweat Index), was assessed. Results showed a significantly dissociation by group between subjective self-perception of physiological arousal and self-perception of affective arousal in questionnaire scores, during adaptation period exclusively. The results don’t show significant correlations among groups between the subjective self-perception of activation and the autonomous reactivity. These results are discussed in terms of their alexithymic characteristics they are associated with autonomic arousal.

Respuestas subjetivas y autonómicas al estrés en la alexitimia. La alexitimia describe una alteración en el procesamiento emocional, manifestada mediante dificultades en la identificación y expresión emocional. Este trabajo examina la relación entre los patrones fisiológicos autónomos y subjetivos de activación en respuesta a una situación de estrés inducido experimentalmente, y el nivel de alexitimia, evaluado mediante la Escala de Alexitimia de Toronto, TAS-20. El experimento consta de seis fases: I-adaptación, II-relajación, III-estrés (aritmético), IV- relajación, V-estrés (visual), y VI-relajación. Durante el procedimiento experimental se registró tanto la percepción subjetiva de activación como la reactividad autónoma, empleando el Índice de Sudoración Palmar. Los resultados muestran diferencias significativas entre la percepción subjetiva de activación fisiológica y afectiva en función del nivel de alexitimia, exclusivamente durante el periodo de adaptación. Asimismo, no aparecen diferencias significativas entre la percepción subjetiva de activación y la reactividad autónoma. Los resultados se discuten en términos de la vinculación de la alexitimia con la activación autonómica.

PDF

The term Alexithymia (literally from Greek: lack of words for emotions), coined by Sifneos (1973) twenty years ago, refers to a specific disturbance in affective-emotional processing that is manifested through the following salient features: 1) difficulty in identifying and describing feelings and emotions verbally, 2) difficulty in distinguishing between feelings and somatic sensations that accompany emotional arousal, and 3) externally-oriented thinking and impaired symbolic activity (Taylor, 2000; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997). The most recent research has stressed the point that in alexithymia there is not only a difficulty in expressing emotions verbally but also a deficit in cognitive processing of emotions (Berenbaum & Prince, 1994; Jessimer & Markham, 1997; Martínez-Sánchez & Marín, 1997; Parker, Taylor, & Bagby, 1993; Roedema & Simons, 1999; Suslow, 1998); as a consequence, emotions remain undifferentiated and poorly regulated (Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1991).

These characteristics are conceptualized both as an affect-deficit disorder and a continuous personality trait that correlates positively with neuroticism (Pandey & Mandal, 1996), depression (Hendryx, Haviland, & Shaw, 1991) and anxiety (Bagby, Taylor, & Atkinson, 1988). Some authors (Horton, Gewirtz, & Kreutter, 1992) argue that alexithymia could be considered also as a state consequent to depression and/or anxiety (Hendryx, Haviland, Shaw, & Henry, 1994), as well as the effect of some chronic psychopathologic and somatic disorders.

It has been hypothesized that the limited emotional awareness and cognitive processing of affects lead to prolonged and amplifies the physiological arousal and neurovegetative reactivity to stress (Infrasca, 1997; Martin & Pihl, 1986; Martínez-Sánchez, 1999; Papciak, Feurstein, & Spiegel, 1985) potentially disturbing the autonomic, pituitary-adrenal, and immune system. Dysregulation or heightened activation of the autonomic nervous system might explain the proneness to «functional» somatic disorder of individuals described as alexithymic. In addition, alexithymia is regarded as one of several possible risk factors that seem to increase the susceptibility to organic disease, certain types of unhealthy behavior, and a biased perception and reporting of somatic sensations and symptoms (Lumley, Stettner, & Wehmer, 1996; Lumley, Tomakowsky, & Torosian, 1997).

Several reports reveal a higher prevalence of alexithymic characteristics among patients with stress-related disorders in comparison with other patients and normal controls (Krystal, Giller & Cicchetti, 1986; Kohn et al., 1994; Shipko, 1982; Zeitlin, McNally & Cassiday, 1993); however, the relationship between alexithymia and stress-related illness is more complex than a simple co-occurrence (Martin & Pihl, 1985).

The stress-alexithymia hypothesis (Martin & Pihl, 1985, 1986a, 1986b; Martin et al., 1986) proposes that «the presence of alexithymic characteristics influences an individual’s response to a stressful situation in such a way so as to create conditions favourable to the development of a stress-related disorder. Perhaps the most important result of the influence of alexithymic characteristics is an aggravated physiological response to stress» (Martin & Pihl, 1986b; p. 108). Several empirical reports, carried out for Martin and Pihl and their colleagues, confirm empirically the influence of alexithymic characteristics on the stress responses.

Several studies they have examined autonomic activity associated with alexithymia, both at rest and in response to stressors. There is some evidence that tonic physiological hyperarousal in association with alexithymia (Papciack, Feurestein & Spiegel, 1985; Rabavilas, 1987; Newton & Contrada, 1994). In contrast, the hypoarousal theory of alexithymia predicts that, under conditions of comparable emotional provocation, there is less physiological activation in individuals with alexithymic tendencies; these is no evidence that alexithymia leads to excessive reactivity to stressor; indeed, most studies found either no alexithymia effect, or that alexithymia was related to less reactivity (Hyer, Woods & Boudewyns, 1990; Linden, Lenz & Stossel, 1996; Nemiah, Sifneos & Apfel-Savitz, 1977; Wehmer, Brejnak, Lumley & Stettner, 1995).

Because the studies using the SSPS and MMPI-based alexithymia scales are now considered to possess insufficient reliability and validity it was considered possible that the opposite results might be a function of different alexithymia measures.

Martin & Pihl (1986a) argued that alexithymic are not necessarily more physiologically reactive to stress per se, but their subjective stress responses tend to be «decoupled» from their autonomic responses. Experimental evidence for a decoupling of HR and the subjective report of tension was found in alexithymics (Martin & Pihl, 1986a; Näring & van der Staak, 1995; Papciack, Feurestein & Spiegel, 1985).

On the basis of this fact, the purpose of the present study was to evaluate the relations to subjective and physiological stress responses related with alexithymia; besides the possible dissociation between the subjective awareness of autonomic arousal and physiological stress responses, in college student with high versus low alexithymia scores. Alexithymia was assessed utilizing the Toronto Alexithymia Scale -TAS-20-. To produce emotional arousal in our subjects, we designed a laboratory stress tasks with four different experimental conditions: baseline (adaptation), relaxation and two conditions of stress, cognitive and visual stimuli.

Method

Subjects

Eighty five female undergraduates psychology students, aged between 18 and 22 years (19.38; Sx = 1.94) participated in this experiment. They all were randomly selected from students enrolled in the «Psychology of Emotion» course at the University of Murcia who scored in the upper o lower quartile of the distribution of scores on the TAS-20. Subjects were divided into two groups according to their alexithymia scores which were bases on quartile criteria, 20 in the lower 25 percentile, 23 in the upper 25 percentile.

All reported being in good health, and none were taking medication at the time of the study that might have influenced either physiological responses or the perception of bodily sensations. Subjects were asked not to ingest alcohol, caffeine, or nicotine 2 hours prior to the experimental study. They all got an academic credit in return for their cooperation.

Materials and measures

The experiment was conducted in a light and temperature controlled laboratory. Electrodermal activity was assessed using the Palmar Sweat Index (PSI); palmar sweat gland activity is a very sensitive indicator of autonomic reactivity (Freedman et al., 1994; Köhler, Weber & Vögele, 1990; Martínez-Sánchez, Fernández & Ortiz, 1998; Turpin & Clements, 1993). PSI was assessed using the plastic impression technique, with fingerprints being obtained at 2,5-min intervals form the left index finger. PSI provides a direct measure of the number of sweat glands using a solution of 3% polyvinyl formal, 1% butylphthalate as a plasticizer, and the remainder ethylene dichloride.

Subjective self-perception of arousal experience during the experiment was assessed using a 10-point scale. Five ratings concerned bodily sensations liked to physiological arousal (racing heart, respiration rate, sweaty hands, muscular tension, and dry mouth). The remaining five scales were included in order to asses affective-subjective responses referred to psychological state, rating were colleted for agited, angry, anxious, nervous and tense. Subjects were asked to rate each symptom on a 10-point scale with a score of 1 representing «not at all» and a score of 10 representing «very much or extremely so»; higher scores indicated heightened perception of arousal.

Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Bagby, Parker & Taylor, 1994), Spanish version (Martínez-Sánchez, 1996). The TAS-20 is a 20-item self-report measure of the alexithymic construct with demonstrated good internal consistency and test-retest reliability (Bagby, Taylor & Parker, 1994), and to three-factor structure theoretically congruent with the alexithymia construct: (F. I.) difficulty to identify feelings and to distinguish between feelings and somatic sensations of emotional arousal; (F. II.) difficulty in describing feelings to others; and (F. III.) externally-oriented thinking. A Spanish version of TAS-20 was accomplished by Spanish psychologists fluent in both English and Spanish, using back-translation methodology; This version showed good internal consistency (Cronbach´s alpha = .78) and test-retest reliability (r = .71, p<.001) over a 19-week interval. These results are comparable to those obtained with the English version of the scale.

Of the various techniques used to measure alexithymia, the Toronto Alexithymia Scale has been most widely used, since multiple studies of its validity and reliability have shown the TAS-20 to have internal consistency, high test-retest reliability, construct validity, and criterion validity (Bagby, Taylor, & Parker 1994; Martínez-Sánchez & Ortiz, 2000). The stability and replicability of this factor structure were demonstrated with both clinical and non-clinical populations by the use of confirmatory factor analysis (Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994; Páez et al., 1999; Parker, Bagby, Taylor, Endler, & Schmitz, 1993).

Experimental tasks and procedure

The experiment protocol (i.e. baseline definition, data reduction recovery periods, etc.) follows the standard designs used in several studies (Köhler, Weber & Vögele, 1990; Köhler & Troester, 1991; Köhler & Schuschel, 1994).

Participants attended individually for the laboratory session. After orientation, Ss was greeted by the experimenter and given a written explanation of the experiment and consent form. After completion of the consent form, the subject sat quietly for 20 minutes. The experiment itself started with a 10 min adaptation phase during which the subjects had to relax and get used to the measurement procedures, especially the taking of the fingerprints (adaptation phase). This was followed by another 7,5 min of relaxation (from min 10 to 17,5), after which there was an instruction phase I lasting 1,5 min; from min 19 to 26,5 (stress I, mental arithmetic), the subjects has to count backward by step of seven as quickly as possible, starting from 2007, which was followed by another resting phase of 7,5 min duration (relaxation II from 26,5 to 34 min). The second part of the experiment followed the same scheme: 34-35 instruction phase II; 35-42,5 (stress II) during this phase the subjects had to watch a distressing film about surgery. Thereafter the subjects were told to relax again (III-relaxation from 42,5-50 min). At the end of all six experimental phases, the set of subjective self-perception of arousal rating was completed immediately.

Data analysis

In order to evaluate the propioception patterns, both physiological and subjective, obtained by the uses of rating scale, regression analysis was carried out using an extension of quasilikelihood for generalized linear models (McCullagh and &Nelder, 1989) usually referred to as Generalized Estimating Equations approach or GEE (Zeger & Lyang, 1986; Zeger, Lyang & Qaqish, 1992; Diggle, Liang & Zeger, 1994; Stokes et al., 1995) with two correlation structures: independent and exchangeable.

With gaussian data, GEE approach is a simple extension of regression analysis for marginal models to take account of nonindependence emerging from the longitudinal pattern of observations within each individual. Zeger & Lyang (1986) proposed some common correlational structures to this end:

(1) independence, which represents an identity matrix and assume no correlation within repeated observations;

(2) exchangeable, which represents association as a matrix with a constant off-diagonal element (the intraclass correlation). It is equivalent to the compound symmetry structure required, but commonly not verified, in using the univariate approach for repeated measures ANOVA;

(3) auto-regressive, which considers association as a matrix with a decreasing correlation depending on the time lag between two longitudinal observations. It is a correlation structure very common in time series analysis;

(4) unstructured, which is a pattern obtained when no restriction are imposed on the structure of association. This is the same correlation pattern assumed in the multivariate approach for repeated measures ANOVA.

In general, GEE can obtain consistent estimates of marginal mean models, and robust standard errors and statistical tests, even if the assumed correlation structure within observations of same individual is mispecified, but statistical tests will be most powerful when the working correlation matrix closely approximates the true correlation matrix.

The estimation procedure may be summarized, from a computational point of view, in the following steps: (a) estimate model parameters for standard (naive) regression coefficient assuming independence of observations, (b) take the residuals from the model and use it for estimate the working correlation matrix within observations of the same individual, (c) Update the regression coefficients using the working correlation matrix obtained in step 2, (d) Iterate until convergence.

The main interest of GEE analysis was focused on the bivariate marginal relationship between the three different response measures used, labeled PHY for subjective self-perception of physiological arousal, AFF for subjective self-perception of affective arousal and PSI for Palmar Sweat Index. Each one of the bivariate relations (PHY-AFF, PHY-PSI and AFF-PSI) was analyzed for all the longitudinal phases of treatment (Adaptation - Relaxation - Stress - Relaxation - Stress - Relaxation, coded from 1 to 6) and experimental group (High Alexithymic - Low alexithymic, coded 1 and 0 respectively), each combination producing a different slope.

Results

Table 1 presents a summary of GEE estimation results for PHY-AFF bivariate relationship assuming independence.

Assuming independent correlational structure (Table 1), PHY-AFF correlation was evaluated in a set of slopes of regression analysis, for each phase and phase x group interaction effect. Significant slopes were obtained for all phases, but only appeared a significant (Z= -2.78; p= 0000) slope for group differences in phase I (adaptation) (Figure 1).

Assuming exchangeable correlational structure (Table 1), PHY-AFF correlation was again evaluated in a set of slopes of regression analysis, obtaining significant slopes for all phases, but only one marginal significant (Z= 1.99; p= 0465) slope was obtained for group differences in phase 6 (III-relaxation).

Table 2 exhibits a GEE estimation results for PHY-PSI bivariate relationship assuming independence (left part) and exchangeable (right part) correlational structure. With independent structure, PHY-PSI correlation decomposition show significant slopes in all phases except phases 2 and 4 (relaxation phases), but no significant slopes for phases x group components. The same pattern of results was obtained with exchangeable structure (Figure 2).

Table 3 exhibits a GEE estimation results for AFF-PSI bivariate relationship assuming independence (left part) and exchangeable (right part) correlational structure. No matter what correlational structure was pointed out, AFF-PSI correlation decomposition shows no significant slopes for all phases and phases x group components (Figure 3).

Discussion

The objective of this study has been to test the relations to subjective and physiological stress responses related with alexithymia; besides the possible dissociation between the subjective awareness of autonomic arousal and physiological stress responses.

Results showed a significantly dissociation by group between subjective self-perception of physiological arousal and self-perception of affective arousal in questionnaire scores, during adaptation period exclusively. The results don’t show significant correlations among groups between the subjective self-perception of activation and the autonomous reactivity (evaluated with the Palmar Sweat Index), although, in general, they correlated poorly with skin parameters. We found that both alexithymic and nonalexithymic subjects showed significant increases in PSI during stress phases. However, contrary to prediction, the groups were essentially the same in their reported affective and physiological states.

Our data confirm Papciak et al.’s (1985) findings. They predicted that alexithymic males college student would react autonomically to stress just as normal subjects do, but, would be less reactive in terms of the subjective report of their emotional state. They used a stress quiz to provoke autonomic responses and the Profile of Mood States rating scale to assess affects, and recorded blood pressure, HR, and frontal electromyogram results. They found that both alexithymic and nonalexithymic subjects showed significant increases in HR and blood pressure during the stress quiz. However the groups were essentially the same in their reported mood states. The only group difference in mood was a significantly higher level of tension reported during the baseline period by the high alexithymic subjects.

No correlations were found between PSI values and self-report of affective and somatic arousal scores. Näring & van der Staak (1995) has demonstrated that high alexithymic subjects perceived heart rate less accurately than low alexithymic subjects. This fact can explain our results partially, and supports the hypothesis that cognitive biases towards experiencing bodily sensations are associated with alexithymia, and that the selective attention to external stimuli does not lead to more accurate identification of autonomic changes.

As regards changes of the self-perception and PSI during stressful situations, our results replicate findings from previous studies (Köhler, Weber & Vögele, 1990; Köhler & Troester, 1991; Köhler & Schuschel, 1994; Martínez-Sánchez, Fernández & Ortiz, 1998; Turpin & Clements, 1993) and are in accordance with reactions in other psychophysiological variables observed in some studies: PSI increased significantly form baseline to stress and decreased after its cessation.

These results confirm partially the decoupling hypothesis (Martin & Pihl, 1986a; Näring & van der Staak, 1995; Papciack, Feurestein & Spiegel, 1985) that it proposes that alexithymia may lead to the inaccurate self-perception of stress states in stress-provoking situations, which may impede the appropriate self-regulation in these situations.

Luminet & Rimé (1998) obtain consistent results with the decoupling hypothesis, their results show that a higher degree of alexithymia was associated with fewer degree of cognitive-experiential level and greater physiological reactivity as indicated by increase heart rate. Recently Näätänen, Ryynänen & Keltikangas-Järvinen (1999) also obtain consistent results with the decoupling hypothesis. The authors conclude that high alexithymic characteristics seem to predispose to the delayed self-perception of physiological stress state so that the beginning of this state may remain subjectively unnoticed and the subjective recovery from it prolonged relative to the physical recovery.

There are some limitations in the present study concerning the generalization of the results. First, the subjects were women and the applicability of the results to the men populations remains to be shown. Second, there may be some limitations in the generalization of the results related with the PSI, further studies are need to replicate changes of PSI during stress, however diverse investigations have demonstrated that PSI displays essentially the same behaviour as electrodermal variables (Clements & Turpin, 1996); the physiologic basis of the electrodermic response has been identified as the activity of the sudoriferous glands, which are innervated by the sympathetic branch, thus making this neurovegetative parameter reliable for measuring arousal. Another problem is the method is intrusive, because the subject perceives clearly the application of the drop, its removal and the social interactions involved.

It must be concluded that our data provides little support with the decoupling hypothesis.

Bagby, R.M., Parker, J.D. & Taylor, G.J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23-32.

Bagby, R.M., Taylor, G.J. & Atkinson, L. (1988). Alexithymia: a comparative study of three self-report measures. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 32, 107-116.

Bagby, R.M., Taylor, G.J. & Parker, J.D. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrente validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 33-40.

Berenbaum, H. & Prince, J.D. (1994). Alexithymia and the interpretation of emotion-relevant information. Emotion and Cognition, 8(3), 231-244.

Clements, K. & Turpin, G. (1996). Physiological effects of public speaking assessed using a measure of palmar sweating. Journal of Psychophysiology, 10, 283-290.

Diggle P.J., Lyang K.Y. & Zeger S.L. (1994). Analysis of Longitudinal Data. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Freedman, L.W., Scerbo, A.S., Dawson, M.E., Raine, A., McClure, W.O. & Venables, P.H. (1994). The relationship of sweat glands count to electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology, 31, 196-200.

Hendryx, M.S., Haviland, M.G. & Shaw, D.G. (1991). Dimensions of alexithymia and their relationships to anxiety and depression. Journal of Personality Assessment, 56(2), 227-237.

Hendryx, M.S., Haviland, M.G., Shaw, D.G. & Henry, J. (1994). Alexithymia in women and men hospitalized for psychoactive substance dependence. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 35(2), 124-128.

Horton, P.C., Gewirtz, H. & Kreutter, K.J. (1989). Alexithymia and solace. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 51(2), 91-95.

Hyer, L.A., Woods, M.G. & Boudewyns, C. (1990). Alexithymia among Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 51, 243-247.

Infrasca, R. (1997). Alexithymia, neurovegetative arousal and neuroticism. An experimental study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 66, 276-280.

Jessimer, M. & Markham, R. (1997). Alexithymia: a right hemisphere dysfunction specific to recognition of certain facial expressions?. Brain and Cognition, 34, 246-258.

Köhler, Th. & Schuschel, I. (1994). Changes in the number of active sweat glands (palmar sweat index, PSI) during a distressing film. Biological Psychology, 37, 133-145.

Köhler, Th. & Troester, U. (1991). Changes in the palmar sweat index during mental arithmetic. Biological Psychology, 32, 143-154.

Köhler, Th., Weber, D. & Vögele, C. (1990). The behaviour of the PSI (palmar sweat index) during two stressful situations. Journal of Psychophysiology, 4, 281-287.

Kohn, P.M. Gurevich, M., Pickering, D.I. & McDonald, E. (1994). Alexithymia, reactivity, and the adverse impact of hassles-based stress. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(6), 805-812.

Krystal, J.H., Giller,, E.L. & Cicchetti, D.V. (1986). Assessment of alexithymia in posttraumatic stress disorder and somatic illness: Introduction of a reliable measure. Psychosomatic Medicine, 48, 84-94.

Linden, W., Lenz, J.W. & Stossel, C. (1996). Alexithymia, defensiveness and cardiovascular reactivity to stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41(6), 575-583.

Luminet, O. & Rimé, B. (1998). Assessing the empirical validity of alexithymia: its predictive value for various levels of emotional responding when exposed to an eliciting situation and when re-evoking it verbally. Paper presented at the 10 th conference of the International Society for Research of Emotion, Wuerzburg, Germany, August.

Lumley, M.A., Stettner, L.& Wehmer, F. (1996). How are alexithymia and physical illness linked? A review and critique of pathways. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41(6), 505-518.

Lumley, M.A., Tomakowsky, J. & Torosian, T. (1997). The relationship of alexithymia to subjective and biomedical measures of disease. Psychosomatics, 38, 497-502.

Martin, J. B. & Pihl, R.O. (1985). The stress-alexithymia hypothesis: theoretical and empirical considerations. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 43, 169-176.

Martin, J.B. & Pihl, R.O. (1986a). Influence of alexithymic characteristics on physiological and sbjective stress responses in normal individuals. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics,45, 66-77.

Martin, J.B. & Pihl, R.O. (1986b). The relevance of alexithymia for research on stress and stress-related disorders. En Humphrey, J.H. (Ed.) Human Stress, current selected research. Vol. 1 (pp. 99-111). N.Y.: AMS Press, Inc.

Martin, J.B. Pihl, R.O. Young, S.N., Ervin, F.R. & Tourjman, S.V. (1986). Prediction of alexithymic characteristics from physiological, personality, and subjective measures. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 45(3), 133-140.

Martínez-Sánchez, F. & Marín, J. (1997). Influencia del nivel de alexitimia en el procesamiento de estímulos emocionales en una tarea Stroop. Psicothema, 9(3), 519-527.

Martínez-Sánchez, F. (1996). Adaptación española de la Escala de Alexitimia de Toronto (TAS-20). Clínica y Salud, 7(1), 19-32.

Martínez-Sánchez, F. (1999). La alexitimia: un factor de riesgo para el padecimiento de los efectos patógenos del estrés. In E.G. Fernández-Abascal y F. Palmero (Eds.). Emoción y Salud (pp. 387-401). Barcelona: Ariel.

Martínez-Sánchez, F. & Ortiz, B. (2000). La evaluación de la alexitimia. In M. Casullo & Páez, D. (Ed.) Alexitimia y cultura. Buenos Aires: Paidós.

Martínez-Sánchez, F., Fernández-Castro, J. & Ortiz, B. (1998). El Índice de Sudoración Palmar: un procedimiento de evaluación de la reactividad autónoma en el estrés inducido experimentalmente. Ansiedad y Estrés, 4(2-3), 227-238.

McCullagh P. & Nelder JA. (1989). Generalized linear models. New York: Chapman and Hall.

Näätänen, P., Ryynänen, A. & Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (1999). The influence of alexithymic characteristics on the self-perception and facial expression of a pshysiological stress state. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 68, 252-262.

Näring, G.W.B. & van der Staak, C.P.F. (1995). Perception of heart rate and blood pressure: the role of alexithymia and anxiety. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 63, 193-200.

Nemiah, J.C., Sifneos, P.E. & Apfel-Savitz , R. (1977). A comparison of the oxygen consumption of normal and alexithymic subjects in response to affect-provoking thoughts. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 28, 167-171.

Newton, T.L. & Contrada, R.J. (1994). Alexithymia and repression: contrasting emotion-focused coping styles. Psychosomatic Medicine, 56, 457-462.

Páez, D., Martínez-Sánchez, F. Velasco, C. Mayordomo, S., Fernández, I. & Blanco, A. (1999). Validez psicométrica de la Escala de Alexitimia de Toronto: un estudio transcultural. Boletín de Psicología, 63, 55-76.

Pandey, R. & Mandal, M.K. (1996). Eysenckian personality dimensions and alexithymia: examining the overlap terms of perceived autonomic arousal. Personality and Individual Differences, 20(4), 499-504.

Papciak, A.S., Feuerstein, M. & Spiegel, J.A. (1985). Stress reactivity in alexithymia: decoupling of physiological and cognitive responses. Journal of Human Stress, 11, 135-142.

Parker, J.D., Bagby, R.M., Taylor, G.J., Endler, N.S. & Schmitz, P. (1993). Factorial validity of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. European Journal of Personality, 7, 221-232.

Parker, J.D., Taylor, G.J. & Bagby, R.M. (1993). Alexithymia and processing of emotional stimuli: an experimental study. New Trends in Experimental and Clinical Psychiatry, IX(1/2), 9-14.

Rabavilas, A.D. (1987). Electrodermal activity in low and high alexithymia neurotic patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 47, 101-104.

Roedema, T.M. & Simons, R.F. (1999). Emotion-processing deficit in alexithymia. Psychophysiology, 36, 379-387.

Shipko, S. (1982). Alexithymia and somatization. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 37, 193-201.

Sifneos, P.E. (1973). Prevalence of «alexithymic» characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 22, 255-262.

Stokes, M.E., Davis, C.S. & Koch, G.G. (1995). Categorical data analysis using the SAS System. Cary, N.C: SAS Institute.

Suslow, T. (1998). Alexithymia and automatic affective processing. European Journal of Personality, 12, 433-443.

Taylor, G.J. (2000). Recent developments in alexithymia theory and research. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 45(2), 134-142.

Taylor, G.J., Bagby, R.M. & Parker, J.D. (1991). The alexithymia construct. A potential paradigm for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatics, 32(2), 153-164.

Taylor, G.J., Bagby, R.M. & Parker, J.D. (1997). Disorders of affect regulation. Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Turpin, G. & Clements, K. (1993). Electrodermal activity and psychopathology: the development of the palmar sweat index (PSI) as an applied measure for use in clinical settings. In J.C. Roy, W. Boucsein, D.C. Fowles & J.H. Gruzelier (eds), Progress in electrodermal research.(pp. 49-59). New York and London: Plenum Press.

Wehmer, F., Brejnak, C., Lumley, M & Stettner, L. (1995). Alexithymia and physiological reactivity to emotion-provoking visual scenes. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183(6), 351-357.

Zeger SL & Liang KY. (1986). Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes. Biometrics, 42, 121-130.

Zeitlin, S.B., McNally, R.J. & Cassiday, K.L. (1993). Alexithymia in victims of sexual assault: an effect of repeated traumatization?. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150(4), 661-663.

Aceptado el 20 de octubre de 2000

Factor de Impacto 2022:  JCR WOS 2022:  FI = 3.6 (Q2);  JCI = 1.21 (Q1) / SCOPUS 2022:  SJR = 1.097;  CiteScore = 6.4 (Q1)